Blueprint for a Global Village: Difference between revisions

From P2P Foundation
Jump to navigation Jump to search
unknown (talk)
(Created page with " '''* Article: Blueprint for the Global Village. By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen. Cliodynamics: SOCIAL EVOLUTION FORUM.''' URL = https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6x...")
 
unknown (talk)
No edit summary
Line 1: Line 1:
 
'''* Article: Blueprint for the Global Village. By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen. Cliodynamics 5: 123–157 (SOCIAL EVOLUTION FORUM).'''
'''* Article: Blueprint for the Global Village. By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen. Cliodynamics: SOCIAL EVOLUTION FORUM.'''


URL = https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh
URL = https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh
Line 19: Line 18:
By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen:
By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen:


"The  conflict  between  lower-level  selfishness  and  higher-level  welfare  pervades  the biological world (Wilson and Wilson 2007). Cancer cells selfishly spread at the expense of other cells within the body, without contributing to the common good, ultimately resulting in the death of the whole organism. In many animal societies,  the  dominant  individuals  act  more  like  tyrants  than  wise  leaders,  taking  as  much  as  they  can  for  themselves  until  deposed  by  the  next  tyrant.  Single species can ravage entire ecosystems for nobody’s benefit but their own. But  goodness  has  its  own  advantages,  especially  when  those  who  behave  for  the  good  of  their  groups  are  able  to  band  together  and  avoid  the  depredations  of  the  selfish.  Punishment  is  also  a  powerful  weapon  against selfishness, although it is often costly to wield. Every once in a great while, the good  manage  to  decisively  suppress  selfishness  within  their  ranks.  Then  something    extraordinary    happens.    The    group    becomes    a    higher-level organism.  Nucleated  cells  did  not  evolve  by  small  mutational  steps  from  bacterial cells but as groups of cooperating bacteria (Margulis 1970). Likewise, multi-cellular organisms are groups of highly cooperative cells, and the insects of social insect colonies, while physically separate, coordinate their activities so well  that  they  qualify  as  super-organisms.  Life  itself  might  have  originated  as  groups  of  cooperating  molecular  reactions  (Maynard  Smith  and  Szathmary  1995, 1999).  Only  recently  have  scientists  begun  to  realize  that  human  evolution  represents  a  similar  transition.  In  most  primate  species,  members  of  groups  cooperate  to  a  degree  but  are  also  each  other’s  main  rivals.  Our  ancestors  evolved to suppress self-serving behaviors that are destructive for the group, at least  for  the  most  part,  so  that  the  main  way  to  succeed  was  as  a  group.  Teamwork became the signature adaptation of our species (Boehm 2011).    Extant  hunter-gatherer  societies  still  reflect  the  kind  of  teamwork  that  existed among our ancestors for thousands of generations. Individuals cannot achieve  high  status  by  throwing  their  weight  around,  but only by cultivating a good  reputation  among  their  peers.  Most  of  human  moral  psychology—including  its  other-oriented  elements  such  as  solidarity,  love,  trust,  empathy,  and  sympathy,  and  its  coercive  elements,  such  as  social  norms  enforced  by  punishment—can  be  understood  as  products  of  genetic  evolution  operating  among groups, favoring those that exhibited the greatest teamwork.
...
Human teamwork  also  acquired  a  mental  dimension  including  an  ability  to  transmit  learned  information  across  generations  that  surpasses  any  other  species.  This  enabled our ancestors to adapt to their environments much more quickly than by the slow process of genetic evolution. They spread over the globe, occupying all  climatic  zones  and  hundreds  of  ecological  niches  (Pagel  and  Mace  2004). The diversity of human cultures is the cultural equivalent of the major genetic adaptive  radiations  in  dinosaurs,  birds,  and  mammals.  The  invention  of  agriculture  initiated  a  positive  feedback  process  between  population  size  and  the ability to produce food leading to the mega-societies of today.  Cultural evolution differs from genetic evolution in important respects but not in the problem that lurks at every rung of the social ladder (Richerson and Boyd  2005).  Just  like  genetic  traits,  cultural  traits  can  spread  by  benefitting  lower-level units at the expense of the higher-level good—or by contributing to the  higher-level  good.  There  can  be  cultural  cancers  that  are  no  less  parasitic than physiological  cancers. For  teamwork  to  exist  at  any  given  rung  of  the  social ladder, there must be mechanisms that hold the wolves of selfishness at bay.  A nation or the global village is no different in this respect than a human village, a hunter-gatherer group, an ant colony, a multi-cellular organism, or a nucleated cell.
...
Accomplishing teamwork at the level of a nation is hard enough, but it isn’t good  enough  because  there  is  one  more  rung  in  the  social  ladder.  Although  many nations have a long way to go before they serve their own citizens well, a nation can be as good as gold to its own citizens and still be a selfish member of  the  global  village.  In  fact,  there  are  many  examples  in  the  international  arena,  where  nations  protect  their  own  perceived  interests  at  the expense  of  the  common  global  future.
..
Moral indignation works at the scale of villages because it is backed up by an arsenal of social control mechanisms so spontaneous that we hardly know it is  there.  The  most  strongly  regulated  groups  in  the  world  are  small  groups,  thanks to countless generations of genetic and cultural evolution that make us the trusting and cooperative species that we are. The  idea  that  trust  requires  social  control  is  paradoxical  because  social  control  is  not  trusting.  Nevertheless,  social  control  creates  an  environment  in  which trust can flourish. When we know that others cannot harm us, thanks to a  strong  system  of  social  controls,  then  we  can  express  our  positive  emotions  and actions toward others to their full extent: helping because we want to, not because we are forced to. When we feel threatened by those around us, due to a lack  of  social  control,  we  withhold  our  positive  emotions  and  actions  like  a  snail withdrawing into its shell. This is why people refrain from unethical acts—to the extent that they do—in  village-sized  groups  and  why  cooperation  is  accompanied  by  positive  emotions  such  as  solidarity,  empathy,  and  trust.  The  reason  that  nations  and  other large social entities such as corporations openly engage in unethical acts is because social controls are weaker and are not sufficient to hold the wolves of selfishness at bay. This is why politicians can talk openly about national self-interest  as  if  nothing  else  matters—even  though  a  villager  who  talked  in  a  comparable fashion would be regarded as insane.
...
In  this  essay,  we  have  sketched  a  surprisingly  simple  solution  to  the  apparent  conflict between self-interest and mutual benefits at all hierarchical levels. We are  suggesting  that  the  social  dynamics  that  take  place  naturally  and  spontaneously in villages can be scaled up to prevent the ethical transgressions that  routinely  take  place  at  a  large  scale.  Why  is  such  a  simple  solution  not  more  widely  known  and  discussed?  Although  we  immediately  realize  this  solution  when  it  comes  to  cell-organism  relationships  or  individuals  within  villages, we do not realize that the same principles also hold for companies or nations.  One  reason  is  because  of  an  alternative  narrative  that  pretends  that  the only social responsibility of a company is to maximize its bottom line. Free markets  will  ensure  that  society  benefits  as  a  result.  This  narrative  makes  it  seem  reasonable  to  eliminate  social  controls—precisely  the  opposite  of  what needs to be done. Governments have been under the spell of this narrative for nearly  50  years  despite  a  flimsy  scientific  foundation  and  ample  evidence  for  its  harmful  effects.  We  can  break  the  spell  of  the  old  narrative  by  noting  something  that  will  appear  utterly  obvious  in  retrospect:  The  unregulated  pursuit  of  self-interest  is  cancerous  at  all  scales.  To  create  a  global  village,  we  must look to real villages."


(https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh)
(https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh)


[[Category:Cooperaation]]
[[Category:Cooperation]]
[[Category:Global Governance]]
[[Category:P2P Theory]]

Revision as of 06:10, 16 August 2021

* Article: Blueprint for the Global Village. By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen. Cliodynamics 5: 123–157 (SOCIAL EVOLUTION FORUM).

URL = https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh


Description

"Life consists of units within units. In the biological world, we have genes, individuals, groups, species, and ecosystems—all nested within the biosphere. In the human world, we have genes, individuals, families, villages and cities, provinces, and nations—all nested within the global village. In both worlds, a problem lurks at every rung of the ladder: a potential conflict between the interests of the lower-level units and the welfare of the higher-level units(Wilson 2015). What’s good for me can be bad for my family. What’s good for my family can be bad for my village, and so on, all the way up to what’s good for my nation can be bad for the global village. For most of human existence, until a scant 10 or 15 thousand years ago, the human ladder was truncated. All groups were small groups whose members knew each other as individuals. These groups were loosely organized into tribes of a few thousand people, but cities, provinces, and nations were unknown (Diamond 2013). Today, over half the earth’s population resides in cities and the most populous nations teem with billions of people, but groups the size of villages still deserve a special status. They are the social units that we are genetically adapted to live within and they can provide a blueprint for larger social units, including the largest of them all—the global village of nations."

(https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh)


Discussion

The Conflict Between Lower-Level Selfishness and Higher-Level Welfare

By David Sloan Wilson and Dag Olav Hessen:

"The conflict between lower-level selfishness and higher-level welfare pervades the biological world (Wilson and Wilson 2007). Cancer cells selfishly spread at the expense of other cells within the body, without contributing to the common good, ultimately resulting in the death of the whole organism. In many animal societies, the dominant individuals act more like tyrants than wise leaders, taking as much as they can for themselves until deposed by the next tyrant. Single species can ravage entire ecosystems for nobody’s benefit but their own. But goodness has its own advantages, especially when those who behave for the good of their groups are able to band together and avoid the depredations of the selfish. Punishment is also a powerful weapon against selfishness, although it is often costly to wield. Every once in a great while, the good manage to decisively suppress selfishness within their ranks. Then something extraordinary happens. The group becomes a higher-level organism. Nucleated cells did not evolve by small mutational steps from bacterial cells but as groups of cooperating bacteria (Margulis 1970). Likewise, multi-cellular organisms are groups of highly cooperative cells, and the insects of social insect colonies, while physically separate, coordinate their activities so well that they qualify as super-organisms. Life itself might have originated as groups of cooperating molecular reactions (Maynard Smith and Szathmary 1995, 1999). Only recently have scientists begun to realize that human evolution represents a similar transition. In most primate species, members of groups cooperate to a degree but are also each other’s main rivals. Our ancestors evolved to suppress self-serving behaviors that are destructive for the group, at least for the most part, so that the main way to succeed was as a group. Teamwork became the signature adaptation of our species (Boehm 2011). Extant hunter-gatherer societies still reflect the kind of teamwork that existed among our ancestors for thousands of generations. Individuals cannot achieve high status by throwing their weight around, but only by cultivating a good reputation among their peers. Most of human moral psychology—including its other-oriented elements such as solidarity, love, trust, empathy, and sympathy, and its coercive elements, such as social norms enforced by punishment—can be understood as products of genetic evolution operating among groups, favoring those that exhibited the greatest teamwork.

...

Human teamwork also acquired a mental dimension including an ability to transmit learned information across generations that surpasses any other species. This enabled our ancestors to adapt to their environments much more quickly than by the slow process of genetic evolution. They spread over the globe, occupying all climatic zones and hundreds of ecological niches (Pagel and Mace 2004). The diversity of human cultures is the cultural equivalent of the major genetic adaptive radiations in dinosaurs, birds, and mammals. The invention of agriculture initiated a positive feedback process between population size and the ability to produce food leading to the mega-societies of today. Cultural evolution differs from genetic evolution in important respects but not in the problem that lurks at every rung of the social ladder (Richerson and Boyd 2005). Just like genetic traits, cultural traits can spread by benefitting lower-level units at the expense of the higher-level good—or by contributing to the higher-level good. There can be cultural cancers that are no less parasitic than physiological cancers. For teamwork to exist at any given rung of the social ladder, there must be mechanisms that hold the wolves of selfishness at bay. A nation or the global village is no different in this respect than a human village, a hunter-gatherer group, an ant colony, a multi-cellular organism, or a nucleated cell.

...

Accomplishing teamwork at the level of a nation is hard enough, but it isn’t good enough because there is one more rung in the social ladder. Although many nations have a long way to go before they serve their own citizens well, a nation can be as good as gold to its own citizens and still be a selfish member of the global village. In fact, there are many examples in the international arena, where nations protect their own perceived interests at the expense of the common global future.

..

Moral indignation works at the scale of villages because it is backed up by an arsenal of social control mechanisms so spontaneous that we hardly know it is there. The most strongly regulated groups in the world are small groups, thanks to countless generations of genetic and cultural evolution that make us the trusting and cooperative species that we are. The idea that trust requires social control is paradoxical because social control is not trusting. Nevertheless, social control creates an environment in which trust can flourish. When we know that others cannot harm us, thanks to a strong system of social controls, then we can express our positive emotions and actions toward others to their full extent: helping because we want to, not because we are forced to. When we feel threatened by those around us, due to a lack of social control, we withhold our positive emotions and actions like a snail withdrawing into its shell. This is why people refrain from unethical acts—to the extent that they do—in village-sized groups and why cooperation is accompanied by positive emotions such as solidarity, empathy, and trust. The reason that nations and other large social entities such as corporations openly engage in unethical acts is because social controls are weaker and are not sufficient to hold the wolves of selfishness at bay. This is why politicians can talk openly about national self-interest as if nothing else matters—even though a villager who talked in a comparable fashion would be regarded as insane.

...

In this essay, we have sketched a surprisingly simple solution to the apparent conflict between self-interest and mutual benefits at all hierarchical levels. We are suggesting that the social dynamics that take place naturally and spontaneously in villages can be scaled up to prevent the ethical transgressions that routinely take place at a large scale. Why is such a simple solution not more widely known and discussed? Although we immediately realize this solution when it comes to cell-organism relationships or individuals within villages, we do not realize that the same principles also hold for companies or nations. One reason is because of an alternative narrative that pretends that the only social responsibility of a company is to maximize its bottom line. Free markets will ensure that society benefits as a result. This narrative makes it seem reasonable to eliminate social controls—precisely the opposite of what needs to be done. Governments have been under the spell of this narrative for nearly 50 years despite a flimsy scientific foundation and ample evidence for its harmful effects. We can break the spell of the old narrative by noting something that will appear utterly obvious in retrospect: The unregulated pursuit of self-interest is cancerous at all scales. To create a global village, we must look to real villages."

(https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6xw505xh)